your approval gives me much gratification - definizione. Che cos'è your approval gives me much gratification
Diclib.com
Dizionario ChatGPT
Inserisci una parola o una frase in qualsiasi lingua 👆
Lingua:

Traduzione e analisi delle parole tramite l'intelligenza artificiale ChatGPT

In questa pagina puoi ottenere un'analisi dettagliata di una parola o frase, prodotta utilizzando la migliore tecnologia di intelligenza artificiale fino ad oggi:

  • come viene usata la parola
  • frequenza di utilizzo
  • è usato più spesso nel discorso orale o scritto
  • opzioni di traduzione delle parole
  • esempi di utilizzo (varie frasi con traduzione)
  • etimologia

Cosa (chi) è your approval gives me much gratification - definizione

PSYCHOLOGICAL CONCEPT DESCRIBING A PROCESS THAT THE SUBJECT UNDERGOES WHEN THE SUBJECT RESISTS THE TEMPTATION OF AN IMMEDIATE REWARD IN PREFERENCE FOR A LATER REWARD
Instant gratification; Quick Fix Mentality; Delay gratification; Deferred Gratification; Delaying gratification; Deferred gratification; Delay of gratification; Delayed Gratification; Delayed reward

Approval proofer         
PREPRESS
Kodak approval
Within the printing industry, the Approval proofer, also known as the Approval Digital Imaging System or Kodak Approval System, was designed for use in Prepress proofing, especially for the highest quality contract proofs.Contract Proof
Much Ado About Nothing         
  • Robert Smirke]] (n.d.)
  • ''Beatrice, Hero and Ursula'', John Jones, after [[Henry Fuseli]] (c. 1771)
  • John Sutcliffe]]: Beatrice overhears Hero and Ursula.
  • fr}}, 1770
  • [[John Gielgud]] and [[Margaret Leighton]] in the 1959 Broadway production of ''Much Ado About Nothing''
  • ''Hero'', [[John William Wright]] (c.1849)
  • Print of [[Ellen Terry]] as Beatrice and [[Henry Irving]] as Benedick in an 1887 performance of the play
  • [[John Gielgud]] as Benedick in a 1959 production
  • Drawing of [[Herbert Beerbohm Tree]] as Benedick and [[Winifred Emery]] as Beatrice in a 1905 production. Act II, Scene v: "Kill Claudio".
  • Swooning of Hero in the Church scene by [[Alfred Elmore]]
COMEDY PLAY BY WILLIAM SHAKESPEARE
Much Ado about Nothing; Conrade; Borachio; Benedick; Much adoe about nothing; MAAN; Governor Leonato; Much Adoe About Nothing; Much ado about nothing; Much Ado About Nothing (title); Bendick and Beatrice
Much Ado About Nothing is a comedy by William Shakespeare thought to have been written in 1598 and 1599.See textual notes to Much Ado About Nothing in The Norton Shakespeare (W.
benedick         
  • Robert Smirke]] (n.d.)
  • ''Beatrice, Hero and Ursula'', John Jones, after [[Henry Fuseli]] (c. 1771)
  • John Sutcliffe]]: Beatrice overhears Hero and Ursula.
  • fr}}, 1770
  • [[John Gielgud]] and [[Margaret Leighton]] in the 1959 Broadway production of ''Much Ado About Nothing''
  • ''Hero'', [[John William Wright]] (c.1849)
  • Print of [[Ellen Terry]] as Beatrice and [[Henry Irving]] as Benedick in an 1887 performance of the play
  • [[John Gielgud]] as Benedick in a 1959 production
  • Drawing of [[Herbert Beerbohm Tree]] as Benedick and [[Winifred Emery]] as Beatrice in a 1905 production. Act II, Scene v: "Kill Claudio".
  • Swooning of Hero in the Church scene by [[Alfred Elmore]]
COMEDY PLAY BY WILLIAM SHAKESPEARE
Much Ado about Nothing; Conrade; Borachio; Benedick; Much adoe about nothing; MAAN; Governor Leonato; Much Adoe About Nothing; Much ado about nothing; Much Ado About Nothing (title); Bendick and Beatrice
n.; (also benedict)
Newly married man, neogamist.

Wikipedia

Delayed gratification

Delayed gratification, or deferred gratification, is the resistance to the temptation of an immediate pleasure in the hope of obtaining a valuable and long-lasting reward in the long-term. In other words, delayed gratification describes the process that the subject undergoes when the subject resists the temptation of an immediate reward in preference for a later reward. Generally, delayed gratification is associated with resisting a smaller but more immediate reward in order to receive a larger or more enduring reward later. A growing body of literature has linked the ability to delay gratification to a host of other positive outcomes, including academic success, physical health, psychological health, and social competence.

A person's ability to delay gratification relates to other similar skills such as patience, impulse control, self-control and willpower, all of which are involved in self-regulation. Broadly, self-regulation encompasses a person's capacity to adapt the self as necessary to meet demands of the environment. Delaying gratification is the reverse of delay discounting, which is "the preference for smaller immediate rewards over larger but delayed rewards" and refers to the "fact that the subjective value of reward decreases with increasing delay to its receipt". It is theorized that the ability to choose delayed rewards is under the control of the cognitive-affective personality system (CAPS).

Several factors can affect a person's ability to delay gratification. Cognitive strategies, such as the use of distracting or "cool" thoughts, can increase delay ability, as can neurological factors, such as strength of connections in the frontal-striatal pathway. Behavioral researchers have focused on the contingencies that govern choices to delay reinforcement, and have studied how to manipulate those contingencies in order to lengthen delay. Age plays a role too; children under five years old demonstrate a marked lack of delayed gratification ability and most commonly seek immediate gratification. A very small difference between males and females suggest that females may be better at delaying rewards. The inability to choose to wait rather than seek immediate reinforcement is related to avoidance-related behaviors such as procrastination, and to other clinical diagnoses such as anxiety, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and depression.

Sigmund Freud, the founder of psychoanalytic theory, discussed the ego's role in balancing the immediate pleasure-driven desires of the id with the morality-driven choices of the superego. Funder and Block expanded psychoanalytic research on the topic, and found that impulsivity, or a lack of ego-control, has a stronger effect on one's ability to choose delayed rewards if a reward is more desirable. Finally, environmental and social factors play a role; for example, delay is affected by the self-imposed or external nature of a reward contingency, by the degree of task engagement required during the delay, by early mother-child relationship characteristics, by a person's previous experiences with unreliable promises of rewards (e.g., in poverty), and by contemporary sociocultural expectations and paradigms. Research on animals comprises another body of literature describing delayed gratification characteristics that are not as easily tested in human samples, such as ecological factors affecting the skill.